By the end of January every year, many people who
made New Year’s resolutions discover that it is hard to keep them. By mid-February, most people who
resolved to change their behavior will find that they have failed and are back
to their old habits.
One difficulty with changing behavior is that you have to
stop doing an undesired behavior, and ultimately replace it with one or more
desired behaviors. As I discuss in Smart Change, your actions are
governed by two brain networks that can be called the Go System and the Stop
System. The Go System engages goals and
promotes action. The Stop System inhibits
behaviors that the Go System has begun to execute. Your ability to stop a behavior successfully,
then, depends on two factors: how strongly the Go System is engaged and how
effectively the Stop System can inhibit an action.
Psychologists have developed a number of laboratory tasks that
can be used to study the Go and Stop systems in action. One interesting task was used in a paper in
the December, 2013 issue of Psychological Science by Julie Bugg and Michael Scullin. This task is called the Prospective Memory
task. Participants are told that they
will see a number of strings of letters (like DUCK or DCUK) and they will have
to respond whether the letters form a word by pressing buttons on a computer
keyboard. This task is called a Lexical
Decision task. However, participants are
also told that there are two special target words (say, CORN and DANCER). If they see one of the target words, they
should press a third button to indicate they saw the target.
After doing that task for 80 trials, participants are then
told that they will continue to do a lexical decision task, but the target task
is now over, so they should only respond whether the letters they see form a
word. Even if one of the target words
from the first part of the study appears again, they should not press the
special key. They key measure in this
study is whether participants mistakenly press the special button when they see
one of the words that had been a target in the first part of the study.
Previous research with this task suggests that the more
times people see the target words in the first part of the task, the more
likely that they will be to mistakenly continue to press the button in the
second part of the task. The idea is
that when you see the words many times in the first part of the task, it
activates the Go System more strongly, and that makes it harder for the Stop
System to overcome the temptation to press the key in the second part of the
task.
In this latest paper, though, the researchers explored an
interesting case. Some participants saw
target words 4 times in the first part of the study. Other participants never saw target words in
the first part of the study. In the
second part of the study, all participants saw words that had been targets 10
times over 260 trials. Surprisingly, 56%
of the participants who never saw targets in the first part of the study
mistakenly pressed the target button at least once, but none of the
participants who saw the target words 4 times in the first part of the study
mistakenly pressed the target button.
What is going on here?
The first part of the study creates the goal to respond to
the target words. When people see those
target words 4 times in the first part of the study the Go System recognizes
that the goal has been achieved, and so its activation is dampened in the
second part of the study. When people
never see the target words in the first part of the study, the Go System
remains active in the second part of the study, which makes it harder for the
Stop System to completely overcome the desire to act.
As a demonstration of this point, another study in this
paper repeated this study. This time,
participants were once again focused on two target words in the first part of
the study. This time, 4 targets appeared
in the first part of the study, but participants saw only one of the two target
words (so half of the participants saw only CORN and the other half saw only
DANCER). In the second part of the
study, participants were much more likely to mistakenly press the special
button when they saw the target that had not
appeared in the first part of the study than when they saw the target that had
appeared.
Why does this matter?
When you try to change your behavior, you probably focus most
on stopping the undesired behaviors. It
may be even more important to find ways to dampen the activity of the Go System
to make the undesired behaviors easier to overcome. One way to do that can be to perform the
undesired behavior a little in order to make the Go System believe that it has
achieved its goal rather than avoiding the undesired behavior altogether.
Obviously, there is a gap between these simple laboratory
studies and the real world. So, there
needs to be more research that helps to translate these intriguing findings
into specific recommendations.