Over the course of your life, attitudes shift. As a teen, you might be a complete omnivore,
diving into every barbecue with abandon.
As a young adult, you might hear economic arguments against meat
production and cut back on the amount of meat you eat. Still later, you might hear about the health
benefits of a plant-based diet and give up meat altogether.
Your attitudes are not just there to be changed, of
course. Your attitudes also help you
decide how to act. Your attitude toward
eating meat affects the foods you buy, cook, and eat.
An interesting paper in the June, 2011 issue of the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
by Dolores Albarracin and Ian Handley explores the effects of wanting to do
something on your attitudes. This
research suggests that wanting to do things makes it easier for you to think
about your attitudes but harder to change them.
In one study, participants were told that they were going to
express their attitude toward gun control at the end of the study. Then, the participants filled in words with
missing letters. For some people, the
words were all related to performing actions (like action, motivation, and doing).
For other people, the words were all related to inaction (like freeze, stop, and calm). A third group filled in words that were
unrelated to either action or inaction. The
idea is that filling in words relating to action would put people in a mode of
wanting to act. Filling in words
relating to inaction would put people in a mode of wanting to rest.
After that, participants expressed their attitude toward gun
control by pressing one button if they were in favor of gun control or a second
button if they were opposed to it. Participants
were fastest to express their attitude if they were primed for action and
slowest if they were primed for inaction.
The control condition came out in between. Another study in this series showed that
people are only faster to express an attitude when they are warned at the start
of the study that they will later have to express that attitude.
A second set of studies examined attitude change. In these studies, people again heard that
they were going to express an attitude about a topic (in this case
vegetarianism). This time, the participants were selected
based on a survey they took earlier in which they expressed opposition to being
vegetarian. Just as in the study I
described earlier, half of the participants were primed toward action and the
other half were primed toward inaction.
Then, everyone read a passage providing a few strong arguments for
reducing meat consumption. After reading
the passage, people gave their opinion about vegetarianism.
After reading a passage promoting being a vegetarian, the
people primed toward inaction changed their attitude toward vegetarianism quite
a bit, while those who were primed toward action did not change their opinion
much at all.
Putting this all together, the studies in this paper indicate
that when people are oriented to act, they have ready access to their existing
opinions. That makes it easier to act,
but the strength of these prior opinions makes it hard for people to change
their existing attitudes. When people
are oriented to wait before acting, they do not have ready access to their
existing opinions. As a result, it is
harder for them to engage in actions.
However, people oriented toward inaction are also more likely to be
affected by information aimed to change their opinion.
This research does suggest why television commercials are so
often effective. When people are
watching TV, they are generally oriented toward inaction. Most TV watching is done by people who are
seated with the goal of being entertained.
As a result, they are in exactly the state they need to be in to be
influenced by persuasive messages.