How do you know when you’re bored? There are probably lots of ways. If you are sitting in a class, lecture, or
talk, you might find yourself feeling frustrated that the speaker is talking in
a monotone. Or, you might not like the
topic. Another possibility, though, is
that you might find your mind wandering to thoughts that have nothing to do
with the class or talk or lecture. Do
you use mind wandering to help you decide whether you are bored?
This question was examined in a paper by Clayton Critcher
and Tom Gilovich in the September, 2010 issue of Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.
How might mind wandering make you think that you are
bored? Perhaps it depends on what you
are thinking about. For example, if you
start thinking about positive things, then you might think that you are
daydreaming about pleasant things because the task you are supposed to be doing
is boring. Often, when your mind wanders
to negative things, it is because those negative things are bothering you, and
you can’t help thinking about them. So
you might assume that negative thoughts come to you whether you want them to or
not, and so daydreaming about negative things might not be caused by boredom.
Another possibility, though, is that you are most likely to
think that daydreaming is a signal that a task is boring when you think about
the kinds of things you could be doing rather than that task. So, when you are sitting in a class or
lecture, you might be most likely to interpret your mind wandering as a symptom
of boredom if you start thinking about other things you could be doing rather
than sitting in that class.
To explore this issue, the authors first found a clever way
to affect people’s mind wandering. They
started the experiment by having people write about other things they could be
doing. Some groups wrote about fun
things they could do. Other groups wrote
about responsibilities they could be taking care of. Later, the group that wrote about fun things
they could be doing daydreamed about more pleasant things than the group that
thought about responsibilities.
Similarly, if people wrote about things they could be doing
instead of being in an experiment, their daydreams tended to be about things
they could be doing at that moment. If
people wrote about things they had done in the past, then their daydreams
tended to be about things in the past.
After doing this writing, people were asked to do something
moderately interesting like doing a jigsaw puzzle or a crossword puzzle. Later, they were asked how much they enjoyed
doing the puzzle.
The results suggest that people use what they daydream about
to help decide whether they are bored.
They said they found the puzzle least interesting when they daydreamed
about positive things they could be doing at that moment. So, if they did the puzzle and their mind
wandered to other fun things they could be doing instead, then they assumed
that they must be bored with the puzzle.
Another interesting aspect of these experiments is that in
one study people were asked to focus on the relationship between what they
wrote about at the beginning of the study and their thoughts later. These people were more likely to realize that
what they daydreamed about was affected by what they wrote about. For these people, daydreaming about positive
things they could be doing did not make them think they were bored by the
puzzle.
In the end, you should remember that your mind wanders all
the time. It can be difficult to focus
attention on one task for a long period of time. And there are many factors that can affect
what you think about when you daydream.
That means that you should not always assume that daydreams are a sign
that you are bored. They might just be a
sign that you have a lot on your mind.